Saturday, 13 August 2016

Should we use brainwave headsets to inform our assessment of students?

You are a committed and qualified ESOL Teacher and I know your sensitivity to and concern for students well-being and cultures being respected.

Can I ask you for your response to the following situation:

Brainwave measuring headsets track beat, gamma, theta and other waves harmlessly and, for the student, sit comfortably on their head much like headphones for listening to music. The headsets are sometimes called bci - brain-computer interfaces.

Do you think that such a headset would be intrusive or culturally offensive or seen to be controlling?  Is the head too sensitive or - in some cultures, sacred - an area for a teacher to be adjusting a headset there?
http://emotiv.com/product/emotiv-epoc-14-channel-mobile-eeg/  
One of the ways of dealing with that may be to have information sessions where adults and students can move around the room touching the headsets trying them on and monitoring themselves (e.g. De Nisco, 2016). 

There are numerous programs for use with the bci headsets such as watching different
      coloured brainwaves react while you concentrate hard or laugh and talk. 

For example, this youtube clip shows a young man playing around with a Neurosky headset and one of the programs that go with it. https://youtu.be/ZnX0aysPFqQ


There are numerous programs to use with bci headsets.

Flight by thought: this is a Neurosky headset. I am  flying a drone by concentrating!! It was sitting on the desk and by focusing my mind on it I managed to get it to take off and fly. You can see they're usually not really intimidating and can be great fun. However there's another issue.

PART 2 of my question:
These headsets can provide data on the resting-state of an individual's brain, when they just sit with their eyes closed for a few minutes.
If for example I had such a headset and put it, one by one, on my students, the results I would garner would include a very powerful indicator of who would learn a second language quickly. The research literature is very clear on this. (e.g.Buchweitz, 2013; Mamiya et al, 2016; Prat et al, 2016).

This is because the brain waves that are being read reflect the structure of the brain. That structure is formed by genetic factors. And by environmental factors like experience. It may seem like a bad thing to know someone is slow in picking up languages, yet that knowledge would also allow us to develop helpful interventions (e.g. DeNisco, 2016). 

One of the main interventions that improves brain connectivity and structure is the learning of a second language. Hence if the brain waves are saying 'slow rate of learning', the prognosis is not necessarily 'slow'. Because learning a second language will help that individual brain become more efficient and thus improve its resting-state indices in the future.

So one can ask

How are these results to be used ?

When is it appropriate to gather them ? 


In the future our learning about our learning will increasingly be informed by technological devices that will support and potentially redefine assessment as depicted by the SAMr model. With bci headsets we stand at a new point in language assessment - shall we use these new devices to assess our students and inform our teaching? They will doubtless only become more inexpensive and more powerful. Perhaps we need to start thinking about this question now.

http://www.elearning.tki.org.nz/Professional-learning/Teacher-inquiry/SAMR-model
To reiterate:

Subjective elements of testing: Our normal behavioural testing gives results for reading comprehension, listening skills, knowledge of grammar and so on.These results are in current use yet they have many subjective elements and in particular do not really indicate the whole scope of the language learning process, the varied skill sets of the student and therefore the student's real "level". (Linck et al, 2013). It is difficult to assess a process as many-sided as language - "such a complex cognitive task such as language learning". (Weiss & Mueller, 2012).

Brain wave readouts from the bci headsets are objective. And they tell us something about the cognitive abilities of the individual at that point in time. They can inform us accurately about the individual's rate of learning a second language. They do not tell us about the quality of knowledge or the future development of the individual in their attitudes or experiences. 



Although these indices are reliable (Badcock et al, 2013) and will for instance repeat in zygotic twins (Prat, 2016), they are not fixed. The three network-level characteristics of the brain which relate to high-level language achievers, as well as general cognitive ability, are efficiency - how well the brain uses or needs to use its resources, synchronization - the degree of efficient connectivity between different networks in the brain and adaptability - how well the cortical system changes to meet different demands. It is important for educators to note that these three characteristics are "genetically based but experientially modifiable". This is discussed by Prat et al in Resting-state qEEG predicts rate of second language learning in adults (2016) .


So the learning of a second language can be the vehicle by which the brain develops enhanced structure for that same language learning. That development in structure also translates to better cerebral resources for other cognitive tasks. Perhaps that could be seen as an encouragement to use the headset devices available to inform our assessment of ESOL students more fully. Yet there may still be hesitations about using bci headsets.

What is your opinion on that?

Your reply will be perused with interest and gratitude.
Thanks

Sue Sullivan
Christchurch, N.Z.


References

Badcock,N.A., Mousikou, P., Mahajan, Y., de Lissa, P., Thie, J., McArthur G.(2013.) Validation of the Emotiv EPOC EEG gaming system for measuring research quality auditory ERPs. Peer Journal 1 :e38

Becker, T.M., Prat, C.S. & Stocco, A. (2016). A network-level analysis of cognitive flexibility reveals a differential influence of the anterior cingulate cortex in bilinguals versus monolingual. Neuropsychologia, May. 85, 62-73. DOI: 10.1016.

Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C. (2013). The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of life reviews, 10(4), 428-443.

Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C. S. (2013). Pushing the boundaries of language in the bilingual brain: A reply to commentary on The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of Life Reviews,10(4), 454-456.

DeNisco, A. (2016). Study of brain waves could answer how learning occurs.  District Administration, May.  http://www.districtadministration.com/article/study-brain-waves-could-answer-how-learning-occurs                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                              Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., Smith, B. K., Bunting, M. F. and Doughty, C. J. (2013), Hi-LAB: A New Measure of Aptitude for High-Level Language Proficiency. Language Learning, 63: 530–566.

Mamiya, PC., Richards,T.L., Coe, B.P., Eichler, E.E., Kuhl, P.K. (2016). Brain white matter structure and COMT gene are linked to second-language learning in adults. Proceedings of National Academy of Science. U S A. Jun 28;113(26):7249-54.

Prat,C.S., Yamasaki, B.L., Kluender, R.A. & Stocco, A. (2016) Resting-state qEEG predicts rate of second language learning in adults. Brain and Language. Jun-Jul; 157-158:44-50. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.007.

Shohamy, E. (2000). The relationship between language testing and second language acquisition, revisited. System, 28(4):541-553.

Weiss, S., Mueller, H.M. (2012). Too Many betas do not Spoil the Broth: The Role of Beta Brain Oscillations in Language Processing. Front Psychol. 2012 Jun 25;3:201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00201. 


Should we use brainwave headsets to inform our assessment of students?

You are a committed and qualified ESOL Teacher and I know your sensitivity to and concern for students well-being and cultures being respected.

Can I ask you for your response to the following situation:

Brainwave measuring headsets track beat, gamma, theta and other waves harmlessly and, for the student, sit comfortably on their head much like headphones for listening to music. The headsets are sometimes called bci - brain-computer interfaces.

Do you think that such a headset would be intrusive or culturally offensive or seen to be controlling?  Is the head too sensitive or - in some cultures, sacred - an area for a teacher to be adjusting a headset there?
http://emotiv.com/product/emotiv-epoc-14-channel-mobile-eeg/  
One of the ways of dealing with that may be to have information sessions where adults and students can move around the room touching the headsets trying them on and monitoring themselves (e.g. De Nisco, 2016). 

There are numerous programs for use with the bci headsets such as watching different
      coloured brainwaves react while you concentrate hard or laugh and talk. 

For example, this youtube clip shows a young man playing around with a Neurosky headset and one of the programs that go with it. https://youtu.be/ZnX0aysPFqQ


There are numerous programs to use with bci headsets.

Flight by thought: this is a Neurosky headset. I am  flying a drone by concentrating!! It was sitting on the desk and by focusing my mind on it I managed to get it to take off and fly. You can see they're usually not really intimidating and can be great fun. However there's another issue.

PART 2 of my question:
These headsets can provide data on the resting-state of an individual's brain, when they just sit with their eyes closed for a few minutes.
If for example I had such a headset and put it, one by one, on my students, the results I would garner would include a very powerful indicator of who would learn a second language quickly. The research literature is very clear on this. (e.g.Buchweitz, 2013; Mamiya et al, 2016; Prat et al, 2016).

This is because the brain waves that are being read reflect the structure of the brain. That structure is formed by genetic factors. And by environmental factors like experience. It may seem like a bad thing to know someone is slow in picking up languages, yet that knowledge would also allow us to develop helpful interventions (e.g. DeNisco, 2016). 

One of the main interventions that improves brain connectivity and structure is the learning of a second language. Hence if the brain waves are saying 'slow rate of learning', the prognosis is not necessarily 'slow'. Because learning a second language will help that individual brain become more efficient and thus improve its resting-state indices in the future.

So one can ask

How are these results to be used ?

When is it appropriate to gather them ? 


In the future our learning about our learning will increasingly be informed by technological devices that will support and potentially redefine assessment as depicted by the SAMr model. With bci headsets we stand at a new point in language assessment - shall we use these new devices to assess our students and inform our teaching? They will doubtless only become more inexpensive and more powerful. Perhaps we need to start thinking about this question now.

http://www.elearning.tki.org.nz/Professional-learning/Teacher-inquiry/SAMR-model
To reiterate:

Subjective elements of testing: Our normal behavioural testing gives results for reading comprehension, listening skills, knowledge of grammar and so on.These results are in current use yet they have many subjective elements and in particular do not really indicate the whole scope of the language learning process, the varied skill sets of the student and therefore the student's real "level". (Linck et al, 2013). It is difficult to assess a process as many-sided as language - "such a complex cognitive task such as language learning". (Weiss & Mueller, 2012).

Brain wave readouts from the bci headsets are objective. And they tell us something about the cognitive abilities of the individual at that point in time. They can inform us accurately about the individual's rate of learning a second language. They do not tell us about the quality of knowledge or the future development of the individual in their attitudes or experiences. 



Although these indices are reliable (Badcock et al, 2013) and will for instance repeat in zygotic twins (Prat, 2016), they are not fixed. The three network-level characteristics of the brain which relate to high-level language achievers, as well as general cognitive ability, are efficiency - how well the brain uses or needs to use its resources, synchronization - the degree of efficient connectivity between different networks in the brain and adaptability - how well the cortical system changes to meet different demands. It is important for educators to note that these three characteristics are "genetically based but experientially modifiable". This is discussed by Prat et al in Resting-state qEEG predicts rate of second language learning in adults (2016) .


So the learning of a second language can be the vehicle by which the brain develops enhanced structure for that same language learning. That development in structure also translates to better cerebral resources for other cognitive tasks. Perhaps that could be seen as an encouragement to use the headset devices available to inform our assessment of ESOL students more fully. Yet there may still be hesitations about using bci headsets.

What is your opinion on that?

Your reply will be perused with interest and gratitude.
Thanks

Sue Sullivan
Christchurch, N.Z.


References

Badcock,N.A., Mousikou, P., Mahajan, Y., de Lissa, P., Thie, J., McArthur G.(2013.) Validation of the Emotiv EPOC EEG gaming system for measuring research quality auditory ERPs. Peer Journal 1 :e38

Becker, T.M., Prat, C.S. & Stocco, A. (2016). A network-level analysis of cognitive flexibility reveals a differential influence of the anterior cingulate cortex in bilinguals versus monolingual. Neuropsychologia, May. 85, 62-73. DOI: 10.1016.

Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C. (2013). The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of life reviews, 10(4), 428-443.

Buchweitz, A., & Prat, C. S. (2013). Pushing the boundaries of language in the bilingual brain: A reply to commentary on The bilingual brain: Flexibility and control in the human cortex. Physics of Life Reviews,10(4), 454-456.

DeNisco, A. (2016). Study of brain waves could answer how learning occurs.  District Administration, May.  http://www.districtadministration.com/article/study-brain-waves-could-answer-how-learning-occurs                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                              Linck, J. A., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., Smith, B. K., Bunting, M. F. and Doughty, C. J. (2013), Hi-LAB: A New Measure of Aptitude for High-Level Language Proficiency. Language Learning, 63: 530–566.

Mamiya, PC., Richards,T.L., Coe, B.P., Eichler, E.E., Kuhl, P.K. (2016). Brain white matter structure and COMT gene are linked to second-language learning in adults. Proceedings of National Academy of Science. U S A. Jun 28;113(26):7249-54.

Prat,C.S., Yamasaki, B.L., Kluender, R.A. & Stocco, A. (2016) Resting-state qEEG predicts rate of second language learning in adults. Brain and Language. Jun-Jul; 157-158:44-50. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.007.

Shohamy, E. (2000). The relationship between language testing and second language acquisition, revisited. System, 28(4):541-553.

Weiss, S., Mueller, H.M. (2012). Too Many betas do not Spoil the Broth: The Role of Beta Brain Oscillations in Language Processing. Front Psychol. 2012 Jun 25;3:201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00201. 


Sunday, 3 July 2016

Global trends

Global trends in education: preparing for the future

Despite years at the forefront of education New Zealand educational circles still regards with some hesitancy overseas scholastic achievements - such as China's standardized test results. As Zhao Yong (2014) implies forcing students to perform to tests is internationally the worst path for creativity. In other words we should pay less attention to number crunching and more to trends such as those in Scandanavian countries where homework has been outlawed for younger students and achievement climbing.


New Zealand under the leadership of National has been falling steadily in the results charts. Exactly how much should we worry about that? Should we not be bringing other types of school environments to our students to enable them to collaborate and create freely according to their interests and skills at the pace that suits their development. 

The secondary system in France now lags far behind in creativity and innovation and does not seem to be taking steps to counter this. Whereas Aotearoa is buzzing with energy, KEP endeavour and local centres and universities such as Ministry of Awesome and Mindlab which are leading the way to the future. A future shrouded in questions. But not in fear as we learn that hands across the nations - and the ubiquitous internet allows us to do just that - will be our best way forward.

While some questions about interdisciplinary approaches and collaborative flipped classrooms are being solved the age-old assessment ogre has not been dealt to. Collaborative achievements do not give individuals a workforce recommendation. It may be that systems of badges will become more normal. It is already a measure that sits easily in the psyche of parents and employers, hinting as it does of boy scouts and honest endeavour.

Learning needs to meet the four criteria (Pearson, 2013)of being accessible, affordable, personal yet - and this may be the bigger challenge - completely scalable.




The future is here now. We are able in a moment to connect and share issues and challenges. Even those that most worry us will cede to the force of goodwill that is beginning to burgeon in the internet. Our neighbours are our online community – in political, educational and leisure domains. Long live the future.

References

Pearson. (2013, April 26). Global trends: The world is changing faster than at any time in human history. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=SdZiTQy3g1g

Zhao Yong - World Class Learners. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Wk--J3E8yqc


https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Wk--J3E8yqc

Using social online networks in teaching

Using social online networks in teaching



Activity 6: Using social online networks in teaching and/or professional development
Create a blog post where you critically discuss the use of social media in teaching and/or in professional development in relation to the two following questions:
  1. What are some key features of social media that are beneficial for teaching and learning? Why?
  2. What social media platform do you feel best supports engagement with your professional development? Why?

Using social online networks for learning has become the normal environment for many school children. It may be fraught with dangers as well as troves of information, it may be disputed whether in fact it is distracting (the guardian 28 July 2015) rather than enhancing of students' learning, but it is indisputably here to stay. While digital tools reach across disciplines, countries and ages of learners, it is yet to be seen if longer term, the early uptake of phones and tablets by very young children is productive or damaging. Silicon Valley parents don't seem to think it’s good during the day, banning technology from their schools and out of the lives of some youngsters until they are 14. 

There is some evidence in cerebral development that might support this position (Wallis, 2014).

www.radionz.co.nz/.../what-3-to-7-year-olds-need-to-learn-nathan-mikaere-wallis
12:00 pm on 8 May 2014
Nathan Mikaere Wallis is part of the Brain Wave Trust, and X Factor Education, Christchurch. He has been a lecturer at the Christchurch College of Education, lecturing in human development, brain development, language and communication and risk and resilience. Nathan has a background of working with children in counselling settings relating to domestic violence, sexual abuse and childhood trauma.

But there is one area of education that may reap benefits from digital tools more than any others and that is language learning.

Signal theory and statistical models show us the early language acquisition is a number crunching business that relies on vibrations and other characteristics of speech that we cannot consciously hear. We have never been able to manipulate this data for the benefit of language learners. Yet the possibility is now there with the level of accuracy reached by speech-to-text databases. Applications designed to lift the power of databases into cerebrally designed programmes for language learners may bring major disruption to the way we go about acquiring other languages.

Technology companies like Samsung have developed databases whose performance is far in excess of anything we have seen earlier. Speech recognition and translation to the written word includes complex variables that can be seen in the number of choices the phone will give when decoding speech.  Language teachers, app makers and game developers world-wide have yet to understand what we have in hand.

It may take time before the embedded interests of linguistics departments and other traditional language stakeholders are ready to move towards dealing with a subconscious system of weighted connections that forms the early stage of language learning. And which may provide the best key yet for adult or mature second language learners' easy path to mastery.

Language learners however have grasped a totally different aspect of online communicating. And to a modest extent, teachers have too. That is social apps. 
My class uses kakao for its online media. It's a robust reliable free Korean app that appears to have no qualms about uploading large videos or storing massive amounts of data on your page.  Unlike VLN - Virtual Learning Network - which suffers, notwithstanding the endorsement of Melhuish (2013), from being not user-friendly for busy teachers.

Whether teachers use whatsApp, Line, kakao or others has little bearing on the extremely positive push that casual relaxed chatting brings to the language learning domain.

Universally students of language dislike writing and it is a task greeted often by groans. Yet online in social media platforms, they see themselves in a different non-judgmental, non -assessed space.

Already for native speakers the culture of texting, emails and chats is that mistakes are overlooked, as long as the message is still there. It is this environment that gives to language students the freedom to plunge into conversations without much care about grammar.  Thousands of non-native speakers world-wide now do not hesitate to share their opinions and information in whatever kind of English they can manage. And generally their postings are accepted in the same vein. This is  very advantageous environment for students who have been brought up in a heavily standardised, competitive, silo version of learning.

The online social media is here - chatterers throughout the internet are practising their English at rates of about ten to fifty times more than before I would estimate. Thanks Facebook and the other apps. You've provided a great learning media for language learners.

References
Melhuish, K.(2013). Online social networking and its impact on New Zealand educators’professional learning. Master Thesis. The University of Waikato. Retrieved on 05 May, 2015 from http://researchcommons.waikato.ac.nz/bitstream/han...

Wallis, N. (2014). "What 3 to 7 year olds need to learn."  National Radio New Zealand. 8 May 2014.


Appendix:

Uses of online social media. Examples from kakao.